What Memory Is For: Creating Meaning in the Service of Action
Glenberg, Arthur
Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20, no. 1 (1997): 41-50.
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X97000010
“Why should psychologists interested in language, learning, and memory care about issues such as embodiment of memory? Because, by ignoring them, we have been making a big mistake. Most theories of memory treat internal representations as meaningless symbols such as a string of zeros and ones that “encode” features […], as pointlike objects with no structure […], or as propositions relating intrinsically meaningless symbols […]. Two problems arise from this treatment. The first is the symbol grounding problem […]: How do those meaningless symbols come to take on meaning? The answer is not as simple as referring the symbol to a lexicon, because words in the lexicon must also be grounded. Also, not all of those meaningless symbols are meant to represent words or wordlike concepts; some are meant to represent complex nonverbal displays […]. The second problem is that we have not availed ourselves of a golden opportunity. By treating internal representation as meaningless symbols, we have not thought about the possibility of taking advantage of other forms of representation. Instead of meaningless symbols, suppose that representations have a structure that is lawfully related to the objects being represented. The structure of the representations might then play an important role in determining, for example, what concepts are easily associated, because their structures literally fit together.”
Seligman, Martin.
Annual Review of Medicine 23, no. 1 (1972): 407-412.
Richards, Blake A., Timothy Lillicrap, et al.
Nature Neuroscience 22, no. 11 (2019): 1761-1770.
Glenberg, Arthur
Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20, no. 1 (1997): 41-50.
Crick, Francis, and Christof Koch
Seminars in the Neurosciences, Saunders Scientific Publications, vol. 2 (1990): 263-275.
James, William
The Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods 1, no. 18 (1904): 477-491
Haidt, Jonathan
Science 316, no. 5827 (2007): 998-1002
Haidt, Jonathan
Psychological Review 108, no. 4 (2001): 814
Schwartz, Daniel L.
Cognitive Psychology 38, no. 3 (1999): 433-464
Pinker, Steven
Studies in the Evolution of Language 3 (2003): 16-37
Loftus, Elizabeth F.
Learning & Memory 12, no. 4 (2005): 361-366
Loftus, Elizabeth F.
American Psychologist 48, no. 5 (1993): 518
Posner, Michael I.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 32, no. 1 (1980): 3-25.
Tomasello, Michael, Malinda Carpenter, Josep Call, Tanya Behne, and Henrike Moll
Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28, no. 5 (2005): 675-691.
Tomasello, Michael, Ann C. Kruger, and Hilary H. Ratner
Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16, no. 3 (1993): 495-511
Tononi, Giulio, and Christof Koch
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 370, no. 1668 (2015): 20140167
Minsky, Marvin
Cognitive Constraints on Communication, pp. 175-200. Springer, Dordrecht, 1980
Eagleman, David M.
Current Opinion in Neurobiology 18, no. 2 (2008): 131-136
Kilner, James M., Karl J. Friston, and Chris D. Frith
Cognitive Processing 8, no. 3 (2007): 159-166
Krakauer, John W., Asif A. Ghazanfar, Alex Gomez-Marin, Malcolm A. MacIver, and David Poeppel
Neuron 93, no. 3 (2017): 480-490
Morris, Richard GM, Paul Garrud, JNP al Rawlins, and John O'Keefe
Nature 297, no. 5868 (1982): 681-683
Damasio, Hanna, Thomas Grabowski, Randall Frank, Albert M. Galaburda, and Antonio R. Damasio
Science 264, no. 5162 (1994): 1102-1105
Haim, Lucile Ben, and David H. Rowitch
Nature Reviews Neuroscience 18, no. 1 (2017): 31
Damasio, Antonio R.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences 353, no. 1377 (1998): 1879-1882
Sloman, Steven A.
Psychological Bulletin 119, no. 1 (1996): 3
Seligman, Martin
Psychological Review 77, no. 5 (1970): 406.